Brain Rules: 12 Principles for Surviving and Thriving at Work, Home, and School (15 page)

Years and years

Today, students are expected to know certain things by certain grades. Curiously absent from this model is how durable that learning remains after the student completes the grade. Given that system consolidation can take years, might the idea of grade-level expectations need amending? Perhaps learning in the long view should be thought of the same way one thinks of immune booster shots, with critical pieces of information being repeated on a yearly or semi-yearly basis.

In my fantasy class, this is exactly what happens. Repetitions begin with a consistent and rigorous review of multiplication tables, fractions, and decimals. First learned in the third grade, six-month and yearly review sessions on these basic facts occur through sixth grade. As mathematical competencies increase in sophistication, the review content is changed to reflect greater understanding. But the cycles are still in place. In my fantasy, these consistent repetition disciplines, stretched out over long periods of time, create enormous benefits for every academic subject, especially foreign languages.

You’ve probably heard that many corporations, especially in technical fields, are disappointed by the quality of the American undergraduates they hire. They have to spend money retraining many of their newest employees in certain basic skills that they often think should have been covered in college. One of my business fantasies would partner engineering firms with colleges of engineering. It involves shoring up this deficit by instituting post-graduate repetition experiences. These reinstatement exercises would be instituted the week after graduation and continue through the first year of employment. The goal? To review every important technical subject relevant to the employee’s new job. Research would establish not only the choice of topics to be reviewed but also the optimal spacing of the repetition.

My fantasy shares the teaching loa d between firm members and the academic community, extending the idea of a bachelor’s degree into the workplace. This hybridization aligns business professionals with researchers, ensuring that companies have exposure to the latest advances in their fields (and informing researchers on the latest practical day-to-day issues business professionals face). In my fantasy, the program becomes so popular that the more experienced engineers also begin attending these refresher courses, inadvertently rubbing shoulders with younger generations. The old guard is surprised by how much they have forgotten, and how much the review and cross-hybridization, both with research professionals and younger students, aid their own job performance.

I wish I could tell you this all would work, but instead all I can say is that memory is not fixed at the moment of learning, and repetition provides the fixative.

Summary

Rule #6
Remember to repeat.

• Most memories disappear within minutes, but those that survive the fragile period strengthen with time.

• Long-term memories are formed in a two-way conversation between the hippocampus and the cortex, until the hippocampus breaks the connection and the memory is fixed in the cortex—which can take years.

• Our brains give us only an approximate view of reality, because they mix new knowledge with past memories and store them together as one.

• The way to make long-term memory more reliable is to incorporate new information gradually and repeat it in timed intervals.

Get more at www.brainrules.net/long-term-memory

Some unfortunate souls don’t have the luxury of experimenting. They become suddenly—and permanently—incapable of ever going to sleep again. Fatal Familial Insomnia is one of the rarest human genetic disorders that exists, affecting only about 20 families worldwide. That rarity is a blessing, because the disease follows a course straight through mental-health hell. In middle to late adulthood, the person begins to experience fevers, tremors, and profuse sweating. As the insomnia becomes permanent, these symptoms are accompanied by increasingly uncontrollable muscular jerks and tics. The person soon experiences crushing feelings of depression and anxiety. He or she becomes psychotic. Finally, mercifully, the patient slips into a coma and dies.

So we know bad things happen when we don’t get any sleep. But, considering that sleep occupies a walloping one-third of our time on the planet, it is incredible to contemplate that we still don’t know
why
we need to sleep. Not that there haven’t been clues. One strong hint came about 10 years ago, from a group of researchers who left a bunch of wires stuck inside a rat’s brain. The rat had just learned to negotiate a maze when it decided to take a nap. The recording device was still attached to those wires, and it was still on.

But to understand how this relates to the purpose of sleep, let’s look at what the brain is doing while we sleep.

you call this rest?

If you ever get a chance to listen in on a living brain while it is slumbering, you’ll have to get over your disbelief. The brain does not appear to be asleep at all. Rather, it is almost unbelievably active during “rest,” with legions of neurons crackling electrical commands to one another in constantly shifting patterns—displaying greater rhythmical activity during sleep, actually, than when it is wide awake. The only time you can observe a real resting period for the brain (where the amount of energy consumed is less than during a similar awake period) is in the deepest parts of what is called non-REM sleep. But that takes up only about 20 percent of the total sleep cycle, which is why researchers early on began to disabuse themselves of the notion that the reason we rest is so that we can rest. When the brain is asleep, the brain is not resting at all.

Even so, most people report that sleep is powerfully restorative, and they point to the fact that if they don’t get enough sleep, they don’t think as well. That is measurably true, as we shall see shortly. And so we find ourselves in a quandary: Given the amount of energy the brain is using, it seems impossible that you could receive anything approaching mental rest and restoration during sleep.

Even if the brain doesn’t behave itself bioenergetically, other parts of the body do rest during sleep, in something like a human version of micro-hibernation. That introduces a second puzzle: Sleep makes us exquisitely vulnerable to predators. Indeed, deliberately going off to dreamland unprotected in the middle of a bunch of hostile hunters (such as leopards, our evolutionary roommates in eastern Africa) seems like a behavior dreamed up by our worst enemies. There must be something terribly important we need to accomplish during sleep if we are willing to take such risks in order to get it. Exactly what is it that is so darned important?

The scientist who studied sleepless Randy Gardner made a substantial early contribution to answering such questions. Often called the father of sleep research, Dement is a white-haired man with a broad smile who at this writing is in his late 70s. He says pithy things about our slumbering habits, such as “Dreaming permits each and every one of us to be quietly and safely insane every night of our lives.”

Dement studied many aspects of the human sleep cycle. What he began to uncover was this: “Sleeping” brains, like soldiers on a battlefield, are actually locked in vicious, biological combat. The conflict involves a pitched battle between two powerful and opposing drives, each made of legions of brain cells and biochemicals with very different agendas. Though localized in the head, the theater of operations for these armies engulfs every corner of the body. This fight is sometimes referred to as the “opponent process” model.

As Dement began to define these two opposing drives, he noticed some strange things about the war they were waging. First, these forces are not engaged just during the night, while we sleep, but also during the day, while we are awake. Second, they are doomed to a combat schedule in which each army sequentially wins one battle, then promptly loses the next battle, then quickly wins the next and so on, cycling through this win/loss column every day and every night. The third strange thing is that no one army ever claims final victory in this war. This incessant engagement results in the cyclical waking and sleeping experiences all humans encounter every day (and night) of our lives.

Dement was not working in isolation. His mentor, a gifted researcher named Nathaniel Kleitman, gave him many of his initial insights. If Dement can be considered the father of sleep research, Kleitman certainly could qualify as its grandfather. An intense Russian man with bushy eyebrows, Nathaniel Kleitman may be best noted for his willingness to experiment not only on himself but also on his children. When it appeared that a colleague of his had discovered Rapid Eye Movement (REM) sleep, Kleitman promptly volunteered his daughter for experimentation, and she just as promptly confirmed the finding. But one of the most interesting experiments of Kleitman’s long career occurred in 1938, when he persuaded a colleague to join him 150 feet underground in Mammoth Cave in Kentucky for an entire month.

Free of sunlight and daily schedules, Kleitman could ask whether the routines of wakefulness and sleep cycled themselves automatically through the human body. His observations were mixed, but the experiment provided the first real hint that such an automatic device did exist in our bodies. Indeed, we now know that the body possesses a series of internal clocks, all controlled by discrete regions in the brain, providing a regular rhythmic schedule to our waking and sleeping experiences. This is surprisingly similar to the buzzing of a wristwatch’s internal quartz crystal. An area of the brain called the suprachiasmatic nucleus, part of that hypothalamus we discussed earlier, appears to contain just such a timing device. Of course, we have not been characterizing these pulsing rhythms as a benign wristwatch. We have been characterizing them as a violent war. One of Kleitman’s and Dement’s greatest contributions was to show that this nearly automatic rhythm occurs as a result of the continuous conflict between two opposing forces.

With the idea that such forces are under internal control, we can explore them in greater detail, beginning with a description of their names. One army is composed of neurons, hormones, and various other chemicals that do everything in their power to keep you awake. This army is called the circadian arousal system (often referred to simply as “process C”). If this army had its way, it would make you stay up all the time. Fortunately, it is opposed by an equally powerful army, also made of brain cells, hormones, and various chemicals. These combatants do everything in their power to put you to sleep. They are termed the homeostatic sleep drive ( “process S”). If this army had its way, you would go to sleep and never wake up.

It is a strange, even paradoxical, war. The longer one army controls the field, for example, the more likely it is to lose the battle. It’s almost as if each army becomes exhausted from having its way and eventually waves a temporary white flag. Indeed, the longer you are awake (the victorious process C doing victory laps around your head), the greater the probability becomes that the circadian arousal system will eventually cede the field to its opponent. You then go to sleep. For most people, this act of capitulation comes after about 16 hours of active consciousness. This will occur even if you are living in a cave.

Conversely, the longer you are asleep (the triumphant process S now doing the heady victory laps), the greater the probability becomes that the homeostatic sleep drive will similarly cede the field to
its
opponent, which is, of course, the drive to keep you awake. The result of this surrender is that you wake up. For most people, the length of time prior to capitulation is about half of its opponent’s, about eight hours of blissful sleep. And this also will occur even if you are living in a cave.

Except for the unfortunate members of 20 or so families worldwide, Kleitman, Dement, and a host of other researchers were able to show that such dynamic tension is a normal—even critical—part of our daily lives. In fact, the circadian arousal system and the homeostatic sleep drive are locked in a daily warfare of victory and surrender so predictable, you can actually graph it. Stated formally, process S maintains the duration and intensity of sleep, while process C determines the tendency and timing of the need to go to sleep. Now, this war between the two armies does not go unsupervised. Internal and external forces help regulate the conflict, defining for us both the amount of sleep we need and the amount of sleep we get. We will focus on two of the internal forces, chronotype and the nap zone. To understand how these work, we must leave the intricacies of battle for a moment and explore instead the life of newspaper cartoonists and advice columnists. Oh, and we will also talk about birds.

lark or owl?

The late advice columnist Ann Landers would vehemently declare, “No one’s going to call me until I’m ready!” and then take her phone off the hook between 1 and 10 a.m. Why? This was the time she normally went to sleep. The cartoonist Scott Adams, creator of the comic strip
Dilbert
, never would think of starting his day at 10 a.m. “I’m quite tuned into my rhythms,” he has said. “I never try to do any creating past noon. … I do the strip from 6 to 7 a.m.” Here we have two creative and well-accomplished professionals, one who starts working just as the other’s workday is finished.

About 1 in 10 of us is like
Dilbert’s
Adams. The scientific literature calls such people larks (more palatable than the proper term, “early chronotype”). In general, larks report being most alert around noon and feel most productive at work a few hours before they eat lunch. They don’t need an alarm clock, because they invariably get up before the alarm rings—often before 6 a.m. Larks cheerfully report their favorite mealtime as breakfast and generally consume much less coffee than non-larks. Getting increasingly drowsy in the early evening, most larks go to bed (or want to go to bed) around 9 p.m.

Larks are the mortal enemy of the 2 in 10 humans who lie at the other extreme of the sleep spectrum: “late chronotypes,” or owls. In general, owls report being most alert around 6 p.m., experiencing their most productive work times in the late evening. They rarely want to go to bed before 3 a.m. Owls invariably need an alarm clock to get them up in the morning, with extreme owls requiring multiple alarms to ensure arousal. Indeed, if owls had their druthers, most would not wake up much before 10 a.m. Not surprisingly, late chronotypes report their favorite mealtime as dinner, and they would drink gallons of coffee all day long to prop themselves up at work if given the opportunity. If it sounds to you as though owls do not sleep as well as larks in our society, you are right on the money. Indeed, late chronotypes usually accumulate a massive “sleep debt” as they go through life.

The behaviors of larks and owls are very specific. Researchers think these patterns are detectable in early childhood and burned into the genetic complexities of the brain that govern our sleep/wake cycle. At least one study shows that if Mom or Dad is a lark, half of their kids will be, too. Larks and owls cover only about 30 percent of the population. The rest of us are called hummingbirds. True to the idea of a continuum, some hummingbirds are more owlish, some are more larkish, and some are in between. To my knowledge, no birdish moniker has ever been applied to those people who seem to need only four or five hours of sleep. They instead are referred to as suffering from “healthy insomnia.”

So how much sleep does a person need? Given all of our recent understanding about how and when we sleep, you might expect that scientists would come up with the answer fairly quickly. Indeed, they have. The answer is: We don’t know. You did not read that wrong. After all of these centuries of experience with sleep, we still don’t know how much of the stuff people actually need. Generalizations don’t work: When you dig into the data on humans, what you find is not remarkable uniformity but remarkable individuality. To make matters worse, sleep schedules are unbelievably dynamic. They change with age. They change with gender. They change depending upon whether or not you are pregnant, and whether or not you are going through puberty. There are so many variables one must take into account that it almost feels as though you’ve asked the wrong question. So let’s invert the query. How much sleep
don’t
you need? In other words, what are the numbers that disrupt normal function? That turns out to be an important question, because it is possible to become dysfunctional with too much sleep
or
not enough. Whatever amount of sleep is right for you, when robbed of that (in either direction), bad things really do happen to your brain.

napping in the free world

Given that sleep rhythms fight their battles 24 hours a day, researchers have studied the skirmishes occurring not only in the night but also in the day. One area of interest is the persistent need to take a nap, and to do so at very specific times of the day.

It must have taken some getting used to, if you were a staffer in the socially conservative early 1960s. Lyndon Baines Johnson, 36
th
president of the United States and leader of the free world, routinely closed the door to his office in the midafternoon and put on his pajamas. He then proceeded to take a 30-minute nap. Rising refreshed, he would tell aides that such a nap gave him the stamina to work the long hours required of the U.S. commander-in-chief during the Cold War. Such presidential behavior might seem downright weird. But if you ask sleep researchers like William Dement, his response might surprise you: It was LBJ who was acting normally; the rest of us, who refuse to bring our pajamas to work, are the abnormal ones. And Dement has a fair amount of data to back him up.

Other books

Cabin Gulch by Zane Grey
The Secret Sister by Brenda Novak
Desolation Island by Patrick O'Brian
The Master Magician by Charlie N. Holmberg