Idiot Brain (36 page)

Read Idiot Brain Online

Authors: Dean Burnett

This is one reason why quitting a drug, “going cold turkey,” is so challenging. If you're a long-term drug user, it's not a simple matter of willpower and discipline; your body and brain are now so used to the drug they've
physically altered to accommodate it
. Sudden removal of the drug therefore has serious consequences. Heroin and other opiates provide a good example of this.

Opiates are powerful analgesics that suppress normal levels of pain by stimulating the brain's endorphin (natural painkilling, pleasure-inducing neurotransmitters) and pain-management systems, providing an intense euphoria. Unfortunately, pain exists for a reason (to let us know about harm or damage), so the brain responds by increasing the potency of our pain-detection system, to cut through the blissful cloud of opiate-induced pleasure. So users take more opiates to shut it down again, and the brain strengthens it further, and so on.

Then the drug is taken away. The user no longer has something that made them incredibly calm and relaxed. What they do have is
a super-enhanced pain detection system
! Their pain-system activity is strong enough to cut through an opiate high, which for a normal brain would be agonizing, as it is for a drug user going through withdrawal. Other systems affected by the drug are similarly altered. This is why cold turkey is so hard, and legitimately dangerous.

It would be bad enough if it was just these physiological changes that drugs cause. Alas, changes in the brain also alter behavior. You'd think the many unpleasant consequences
and demands of drug use should logically be sufficient to stop people using them. However, “logic” is one of the first casualties of drug use. Parts of the brain may work to build tolerance and maintain normal functioning, but it's so diverse that other brain areas are simultaneously working to ensure we keep taking the drug. For example, it can cause the opposite of tolerance; drug users become sensitized to the effects of a drug by suppression of the adaptation systems,
20
so it becomes
more
potent, compelling the individual to seek it out even more. This is one factor that leads to addiction.
†

There's more. Communication between the reward pathway and the amygdala serves to provide a strong emotional response to anything drug related, aka “drug cues.”
22
Your specific pipe, syringe, lighter, the smell of the substance, all these become emotionally charged and stimulating in their own right. This means drug users can experience the effects of a drug, directly from the things
associated
with it.

Heroin addicts provide another grim example of this. One treatment for heroin addiction is methadone, another opiate that provides similar (though reduced) effects, theoretically enabling users to give up gradually without going cold turkey. Methadone is supplied in a form than can only be swallowed (it looks like worryingly green cough syrup), whereas heroin is usually injected. But so strong a connection does the brain make between injection and the effects of heroin, that the act of
injecting causes a high. Addicts have been known to pretend to swallow methadone, then spit it into a syringe and inject it.
23
This is an incredibly dangerous act (if only for hygiene reasons) but the warping of the brain by drugs means the method of delivery is almost as important as the drug itself.

Constant stimulation of the reward pathway by drugs also alters our ability to think and behave rationally. The interface between the reward pathway and the frontal cortex, where the important conscious decisions are made, is modified, so that drug-acquiring behaviors are prioritized above normally more important things (such as holding down a job, obeying the law, showering). By contrast, negative consequences of drugs (being arrested, getting a nasty illness from needle sharing, alienating friends and family) are actually suppressed in terms of how much they bother or worry us. Hence an addict will shrug nonchalantly at losing all their worldly possessions but will repeatedly risk their own skin to obtain another hit.

Perhaps most disconcerting is the fact that excessive drug use suppresses activity of the prefrontal cortex and impulse-control areas. The parts of the brain that say, “Don't do that,” “That's not clever,” “You'll regret this,” and so on—their influence is diminished. Free will may be one of the most profound achievements of the human brain, but if it gets in the way of a buzz then it's got to go.
24

The bad news keeps coming. These drug-based alterations to the brain and all the associations made don't go away when drug use stops; they're just “not used.” They may fade somewhat but they endure, and will still be there should the individual sample the drug again, no matter how long they've abstained. This is why relapse is so easy, and such a big problem.

Exactly how people end up becoming regular drug takers varies massively. Maybe they live in bleak deprived areas where the only relief from the realities of life is from drugs. They might have an undiagnosed mental disorder and end up “self-medicating” by trying drugs to alleviate the problems they experience every day. There is even believed to be a genetic component to drug use, possibly due to some people having a less-developed or underpowered impulse-control region of the brain.
25
Everyone has that part of them that, when offered the opportunity to try a new experience, says, “What's the worst that could happen?” Sadly, some people lack that other part of the brain that explains in exquisite detail exactly what could happen. This accounts for why many people can safely dabble with drugs and walk away unchanged, while others are ensnared from the first hit onwards.

Regardless of the cause or initial decisions that led to it, addiction is recognized by professionals as a condition to be treated rather than a failing to be criticized or condemned. Excessive drug use causes the brain to undergo startling changes, many of which contradict each other. Drugs seem to turn the brain against itself in some prolonged war of attrition, where our lives are the battleground. This is a terrible thing to do to yourself, but drugs make it so that you don't care.

This
is your brain on drugs. It is pretty hard to convey all this with eggs, admittedly.

Reality is overrated anyway

(Hallucinations, delusions and what the brain does to cause them)

One of the most common occurrences in mental health problems is psychosis, where someone's ability to tell what's real or not is compromised. The most common expressions of this are hallucinations (perceiving something that isn't actually there) and delusions (unquestionably believing something that is demonstrably not true), along with other behavioral and thought disruptions. The idea of these things happening can be deeply unsettling; losing your very grasp on reality itself, how are you supposed to deal with that?

Worryingly, the neurological systems handling something as integral as the ability to grasp reality are disturbingly vulnerable. Everything covered in this chapter so far—depression, drugs and alcohol, stress and nervous breakdowns—can end up triggering hallucinations and delusions in the overtaxed brain. There are also many other things that trigger them, like dementia, Parkinson's disease, bipolar disorder, lack of sleep, brain tumors, HIV, syphilis, Lyme disease, multiple sclerosis, abnormally low blood sugar, alcohol, cannabis, amphetamines, ketamine, cocaine, and more. Some conditions are so synonymous with psychosis they're known as “psychotic disorders,” the most well known of which is schizophrenia. To clarify, schizophrenia isn't about split personalities; the “schism” for which it is named is more between the individual and reality.

While psychosis often results in the sensation of being touched when you're not being, or tasting or smelling things that aren't there, the most common are aural hallucinations, aka “hearing voices.”
There are several classes of this type of hallucination.

There are first-person auditory hallucinations (“hearing” your own thoughts, as if they're spoken by someone else), second person (hearing a separate voice talking
to
you) and third person (hearing one or more voices talking
about
you, providing a running commentary of what you're doing). The voices can be male or female, familiar or unfamiliar, friendly or critical. If the latter is the case (which it usually is), they are “derogatory” hallucinations. The nature of hallucinations can help diagnosis; for instance, persistent derogatory third-person hallucinations are a reliable indicator of schizophrenia.
26

How does this happen? It's tricky to study hallucinations, because you'd need people to hallucinate on cue in the lab. Hallucinations are generally unpredictable, and if someone could switch them on and off at will, they wouldn't be a problem. Nevertheless, there have been numerous studies, focusing largely on the auditory hallucinations experienced by those with schizophrenia, which tend to be very persistent.

The most common theory of how hallucinations occur focuses on the complex processes the brain uses to differentiate between neurological activity generated by the outside world, and activity we generate internally. Our brains are always chattering away, thinking, musing, worrying and so on. This all produces (or is produced by) activity within the brain.

The brain is usually quite capable of separating internal from external activity (that produced by sensory information), like keeping received and sent emails in separate folders. The theory is that hallucinations occur when this ability is compromised. If you've ever accidentally lumped all your emails together in the same folder you'll know how confusing this
can be, so imagine doing that with your brain functions.

So the brain loses track of what's internal and what's external activity, and the brain isn't good with such things. This was demonstrated in
Chapter 5
, which discussed how blindfolded people struggle to tell the difference between apples and potatoes when eating them. That's the brain functioning “normally.” In the case of hallucinations, the systems that separate internal and external activity are (metaphorically) blindfolded. So people end up perceiving internal monologue as an actual person speaking, as internal musings and hearing spoken words activates the auditory cortex and associated language-processing areas. Indeed, a number of studies have shown that persistent third-person hallucinations correspond with reduced volumes of gray matter in these areas.
27
Gray matter does all the processing, so this suggests reduced ability to distinguish between internally and externally generated activity.

Evidence for this comes from an unlikely source: tickling. Most people can't tickle themselves. Why not? Tickling should feel the same no matter who does it, but tickling ourselves involves conscious choice and action on our part, which requires neurological activity, which the brain recognizes as being internally generated, so it's processed differently. The brain detects the tickling, but internal conscious activity flagged it up beforehand, so it's ignored. As such, it provides a useful example of the brain's ability to differentiate between internal and external activity. Professor Sarah-Jayne Blakemore and her colleagues at the Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology studied the ability of psychiatric patients to tickle themselves.
28
They found that, compared with non-patients, patients who experienced hallucinations were
far more sensitive to self-tickling, suggesting a compromised ability to separate internal and external stimuli.

While an interesting approach (and one not without flaws), please note that being able to tickle yourself does not automatically mean you're psychotic. People vary tremendously. My wife's college roommate could tickle himself, and has never had any psychiatric issues. He's extremely tall though; maybe the nerve signals take so long to get to the brain from the tickling site it just forgets how they originated?
‡

Neuroimaging studies have suggested further theories about how hallucinations generally come about. An extensive review of the available evidence, published by Paul Allen and his colleagues in 2008,
29
suggests an intricate (but surprisingly logical) mechanism.

As you may expect, our brain's ability to differentiate between internal and external occurrences is derived from multiple areas acting together. There are fundamental subcortical areas, predominantly the thalamus, that provide raw information from the senses. This ends up in the sensory cortex, which is an umbrella term for all the different areas involved in sensory processing (the occipital lobe for vision, auditory and olfactory processing in the temporal lobes, and so on). It's often subdivided into primary and secondary sensory cortex; primary processes the raw features of a stimulus, secondary processes more fine detail and recognition (for example, the primary sensory cortex would recognize specific lines, edges and colors, the secondary would recognize all of this as an oncoming bus, so both are important).

Connecting to the sensory cortex are areas of the prefrontal cortex (decisions and higher functions, thinking), premotor cortex (producing and overseeing conscious movement), cerebellum (fine motor control and maintenance) and regions with similar functions. These areas are generally responsible for determining our conscious actions, providing information needed to determine which activity is internally generated, as in the tickling example. The hippocampus and amygdala also incorporate memory and emotion, so we can remember what we're perceiving and react accordingly.

Activity between these interconnected regions maintains our ability to separate the outside world from the one inside our skull. It's when the connections are changed by something that affects the brain that hallucinations occur. Increased activity in the secondary sensory cortex means signals generated by internal processes get stronger and affect us more. Reduced activity from the connections to the prefrontal cortex, premotor cortex, and so on, prevents the brain from recognizing information that is produced internally. These areas are also believed to be responsible for monitoring the external/internal detection system, ensuring genuine sensory information is processed as such, so compromised connections with these areas would mean more internally generated information is “perceived” as genuine.
30

Other books

Out Of The Past by Geri Foster
Mysteries of Motion by Hortense Calisher
October song by Unknown
Playing Dead by Jessie Keane
At Risk by Alice Hoffman
This Time Forever by Williams, Adrienne
Hot Island Nights by Sarah Mayberry