The Mind and the Brain (22 page)

Read The Mind and the Brain Online

Authors: Jeffrey M. Schwartz,Sharon Begley

Tags: #General, #Science

Figure 6: A
. The sensory homunculus depicts the location and amount of cortical space devoted to processing tactile signals from different places on the body. Sensitive regions such as the lips and genitals command a great deal of cortical space.
B
. The motor homunculus shows the amount of cortical space devoted to controlling the movement of different regions of the body. Muscles involved in speech and hand movements receive a great deal of cortex, while less dextrous regions such as the shoulder receive very little.

But it was an odd map. True, the part of the somatosensory cortex that registers sensation from the lips lies between the regions that register sensation from the forehead and the chin. So far, so good. The cortical representation of one finger is positioned relative to those of the other fingers, reflecting the arrangement of the fingers on the hand. Also good. But beyond these basics, the cortical representations of different regions of the body are arranged in a way that makes you suspect nature has a bizarre sense of humor. The somatosensory representation of the fingers, for instance, sits beside the face. The representation of the genitals lies below the
feet. The reason for this arrangement remains lost in the mists of evolution. One intriguing hypothesis, however, is that it reflects the experience of the curled-up fetus: in utero, our arms are often bent so that our hands touch our cheeks, our legs curled up so that our feet touch our genitals. Perhaps months of simultaneous activation of these body parts, with the corresponding synchronous firing of cortical neurons, results in those cortical neurons’ “being fooled” into thinking that these body parts are contiguous. It would be another example of coincident input’s producing coherent structures during prenatal development, as discussed in Chapter 3.

The other oddity of the somatosensory cortex is easier to explain. The amount of cortical territory assigned to a given part of the body reflects not the size of that body part but its sensitivity. As a consequence, the somatosensory representation of the lips dwarfs the representation of the trunk or calves. The result is a homunculus with dinner-plate lips. Our little man also has monstrous hands and fingers: the touch-sensitive neurons on the tip of your index finger are fifteen times as dense as those on, for instance, your shin, so the homunculus’s index finger receives more cortical real estate than a whole leg. The density of touch receptors on the tongue is also more than fifteen times as great as that of those on the back of your hand. Place the tip of your tongue under your front teeth and you’ll feel the little ridges; but place the back of your hand against the teeth and all you’re likely to feel is a dull edge.

The motor cortex, which controls the voluntary actions of muscles moving every part of the body, is also laid out like a homunculus. Here, the amount of neural territory assigned to moving such dexterous parts as the hands dwarfs the amount given to moving, say, the ears. The lips get more motor cortex than the leg; we are, after all, the ape that speaks. The torso is dwarfed by the representations of the face, tongue, and hands. The amount of motor cortex devoted to moving the thumb is as great as the amount zoned for moving the entire forearm: the former is capable of much finer movements than the latter. But the motor homunculus is as jum
bled as his somatosensory brother. Penfield, again using mild electrical stimulation of the exposed brains of surgical patients, discovered that the motor cortex maps out a body plan as cartoonish as the somatosensory cortex does. The representation of the leg sits near the center of the motor cortex, at the crown of the head; working outward, the arm (including hand and fingers), head, and face follow.

Despite a contradictory experiment here and an iconoclast there, for decades it had been axiomatic that there was no plasticity in the somatosensory or motor cortex of the adult brain. The only form of plasticity allowed into the textbooks was that based on Hebbian remodeling, in which neurons that fire together wire together. Since Hebb’s 1949 paper, many studies had demonstrated this limited kind of cortical plasticity, but plasticity in the sense of extensively rezoning the cortex, so that a region that originally performed one function switches to another, was unheard of.

This dogma had profound real-world consequences. It held that if the brain sustained injury through stroke or trauma to, say, a region responsible for moving the left arm, then other regions could not step up to the plate and pinch-hit. The function of the injured region would be lost forever. Observations that challenged this paradigm were conveniently explained away. Faced with the fact that stroke-related brain injury, for instance, is not always permanent—someone who suffers an infarct in the region of the right motor cortex responsible for moving the left leg might nevertheless regain some control of the left leg—the antiplasticity camp didn’t budge. No, it isn’t possible that another region of the motor cortex assumes control of the left leg in such cases, they argued. At best, lower and more primitive regions such as the basal ganglia, which encode grosser patterns of movement, might take over some of the functions of the injured region. But recovery from brain injury, held this camp, in no way undermined the paradigm that neural circuitry in the adult is fixed (except for memory and learning through Hebbian processes). The possibility that the adult brain might have the
power to adapt or change as the result of experiences was dismissed. Sherrington’s “enchanted loom” weaving a “dissolving pattern” seemed to be a whimsical illusion of a more naïve age.

 

As an undergraduate at Oregon’s University of Portland in the early 1960s, Michael Merzenich was pretty sure he wanted to become a physician. But he stumbled onto a different vocation. A Portland alumnus had founded a scientific equipment company called Tektronix; over the years, the alum had contributed entire rooms full of gadgets and gizmos to support his alma mater. Because almost no one knew how to use it all, though, the stuff sat largely untouched. Almost on a lark, Merzenich and a friend decided to see what they could make of it. Even though they were “almost entirely ignorant about what we were doing,” as Merzenich recalls, after a lot of fiddling around they actually managed to accomplish something: recording the electrical activity in the neurons of insects. A professor suggested that Mike call the med school; with luck, he might find someone who would take pity on him and his coconspirator and supervise their Tektronix exploits. Making a cold call, Merzenich suddenly had John Burkhardt on the line. President of the Physiological Society, Burkhardt was a lion of neuroscience. Surprised and impressed at what Merzenich had been able to accomplish, he decided to take the young man under his wing. Eventually, Burkhardt made a few calls for Merzenich; without even applying, Merzenich found that both Harvard and Johns Hopkins University would be delighted to have him enroll in their graduate school. Merzenich headed for Hopkins, whose department had a strong reputation for research into awareness and perception. Although barely into his twenties, Merzenich already knew that his interest in neuroscience stemmed from more than a passionate desire to work out, say, the neuron-by-neuron circuit that enables a fly to move its right front leg. “I had been interested in philosophy,” Merzenich says, “and I looked at neuroscience as a way to address questions of philosophy from a scientific perspective.”

After finishing graduate school in 1968, Merzenich began a postdoctoral fellowship at the University of Wisconsin. There, he focused on how information from peripheral nerves is represented in the brain, and how that representation might change. In his experiment, he cut (“transected”) the sensory nerves in one hand of each of six macaque monkeys. Later, after the tiny, peripheral nerve branches had atrophied, he surgically reconnected each severed nerve where it had been cut. The peripheral branches were left to grow back on their own. The result: skin “addresses” in the brain were shuffled like a deck in Vegas. What happened was that the branches of the sensory nerves grew back into the skin almost randomly, and not necessarily to their original sites, Merzenich reported in 1972. “They sort of meandered,” he explains. The poor brain was hoodwinked. A nerve that used to carry information from, say, the tip of the forefinger had instead grown back to the middle segment of that finger. When a signal arrived in the brain via that nerve, the brain naturally figured it was hearing from the fingertip, when it fact the transmission came from a few millimeters away. Something similar happened at the other end, too: nerves from some skin surfaces took over the cortical representation zones originally occupied by others. As a result, a single skin surface (such as the tip of a finger) came to be represented across several small, separate patches in the cortex, rather than the usual continuous swatch, as its nerves grew back to different regions of the cortex. Normally, adjacent regions within a parcel of somatosensory cortex represent adjacent skin surfaces. But now the skin inputs to these adjacent cortical regions were all messed up.

But not necessarily forever. With enough use of their rewired hands, Merzenich’s monkeys could achieve near-total correction of the scrambled brain addresses. The brain sorted out the new pattern of connections—okay, I keep receiving input from these two nerves at the same time; I’m going to guess that they come from adjacent areas of skin—and remade the somatosensory cortex accordingly. In other words, the brain registers which skin sites fire
simultaneously. Through such coincident sensory (“afferent”) input, the cortex creates functionally coherent receptive fields, a dramatic example of what has come to be called activity-dependent cortical reorganization.

“I knew it was astounding reorganization, but [back in the 1970s] I couldn’t explain it,” says Merzenich. “It was difficult to account for the emergence of such orderly receptive fields when we shuffled the sensory input so drastically. Looking back on it, I realized that I had seen evidence of neuroplasticity. But I didn’t know it at the time. I simply didn’t know what I was seeing.” Merzenich pauses. “And besides, in mainstream neuroscience, nobody would believe that plasticity was occurring on this scale.” Although scientists in James’s and Sherrington’s day had debated and speculated about brain remodeling, by the time Merzenich got interested, the idea had pretty much been run out of town on a rail. Those tidy diagrams assigning one function to this patch of brain and another to that one—here some language comprehension, there some lip sensation—proved too compelling: neurons of the brain, held the dogma, figure out early what they’re going to be and stick to it for life.

Merzenich wasn’t persuaded. He determined to see just how extensively the cortex could reorganize after new patterns of input. What he needed were brains in which the somatosensory cortex is spread over a flat surface, rather than being plagued by fissures and sulci, simply so he could see the thing better. While at Wisconsin, he had struck up a friendship with Jon Kaas, also a postdoctoral fellow there. When Merzenich went off to the University of California, San Francisco (UCSF), in 1971, Kaas joined Vanderbilt University, where he was doing experiments with little New World primates called owl monkeys (
Aotus trivirgatus
); their somatosensory cortex was perfect for what Merzenich had in mind. The squirrel monkey (
Saimiri sciureus
), too, had an easy-to-map somatosensory cortex and would also prove popular in neuroplasticity investigations. In both species, the map of the hand takes up roughly eight to fourteen square millimeters of cortical real estate. Merzenich and Kaas
began to investigate how surgically disconnecting sensory input alters animals’ brains.

“We decided to re-do the experiment we had started together at Wisconsin,” recalls Kaas, “in which we cut one of the monkey’s peripheral nerves, let it grow back, and then examined the somatosensory cortex to see if there had been any changes.” They started with what they figured would be the control experiment: severing the median nerve of an adult monkey’s hand and not reconnecting it (left alone, severed nerves do not mend themselves). Once the monkeys had lived with their severed nerve for several months, Merzenich took a sabbatical from UCSF and joined Kaas at Vanderbilt. The next step was to probe how the surgery altered the animals’ brains. To do that, they recorded the activity in hundreds of locations in the monkeys’ somatosensory cortices. Thanks to new anesthetics, which did not render the cortex unresponsive as old barbiturates did, the team was able to put the animals under anesthesia but still get readings. “We were in the lab all the time,” recalls Kaas.

Mapping takes hours and hours, so we would start in the morning and not leave until two days later, once we had finished all the recordings

or else got too goofy to work. No one wanted to miss out on what we were finding. There was a feeling that you didn’t know what would be seen next, and if you weren’t right there you wouldn’t believe it. I remember taking a candy break one midnight after a pretty successful run, and eating a Payday. When I finally finished at 6
A.M.
one day, I broke out a beer. It was called “Quitting Time.”

Their findings were worth more than a cheap beer, for what the researchers assumed would be the control experiment—preventing the cut nerve from reconnecting—turned out to be a neuroscience landmark. “Quite unexpectedly, the cortex that had received input from the severed nerve, and which should now have been silent,
responded to stimulation of other parts of the hand,” Kaas recalls. Within three weeks after they had severed the median nerve, which carries signals from the thumbward half of the monkey’s palm and fingers, new inputs from the radial and ulnar nerves—which serve the pinky side and the back of the hand, respectively—had completely annexed the median nerve’s cortical territory. After four and a half months, the new maps were as refined as the original: “A beautiful, complete topographic representation of the dorsal hairy fingers [and ulnar palm] emerges,” Merzenich later wrote with his UCSF collaborator, William Jenkins, “almost equal in detail to the representation…that it supplanted.” As the investigators put it in 1983, “These results are completely contrary to a view of sensory systems as consisting of a series of hardwired machines.”

Other books

Her Imaginary Lover by Doris O'Connor
Land of the Blind by Jess Walter
KNOX: Volume 3 by Cassia Leo
Phantom of the Heart by Stein Willard
Slave to Love by Julie A. Richman
Flowers of the Bayou by Lam, Arlene
The Lawless by William W. Johnstone