Read Clinical Handbook of Mindfulness Online
Authors: Fabrizio Didonna,Jon Kabat-Zinn
Tags: #Science, #Physics, #Crystallography, #Chemistry, #Inorganic
training may exert beneficial outcomes. These mechanisms include reduced
rumination
(Segal et al., 2002),
desensitization through exposure to negative emotion
(Linehan, 1993),
and improved ability to behave constructively when experiencing unpleasant emotions or sensations
(Kabat-Zinn, 1982).
Chapter 9 Assessment of Mindfulness
159
Lykins and Baer (in press)
showed that the acting with awareness,
nonjudging
and
nonreactivity
facets of mindfulness completely mediated the rela-
tionships between meditation experience and rumination, fear of emotion,
and ability to engage in goal-directed behavior when upset. Two of these
variables also were shown to partially mediate relationships between mind-
fulness and psychological well-being. Overall, results support the idea that
increased mindfulness improves psychological functioning by reducing rumi-
nation and fear of emotion.
In another recent study,
Carmody and Baer (2008)
administered the FFMQ
to 174 individuals with stress, anxiety, and illness-related complaints who
completed MBSR, an 8-week group program based on the intensive practice
of several forms of mindfulness meditation (see other chapters in this volume
for more detail). Scores on all five facets of mindfulness increased signifi-
cantly from pre- to post-treatment. For four of the facets (all but
describing
)
increases were related to the amount of home practice of meditation exer-
cises that participants completed during the program. Increases in mind-
fulness also were shown to mediate the relationship between extent of
home practice and improvement in psychological symptoms and stress lev-
els. Weaker findings for the
describing
facet may not be surprising in this
case, as MBSR places very little emphasis on verbal labeling of experiences.
In contrast, DBT and ACT include exercises for the labeling of emotions, cog-
nitions, and sensations. Study of the
describing
facet with these interventions
is warranted.
Overall, preliminary evidence from studies of the FFMQ supports two gen-
eral conclusions. First, the five subscales of the FFMQ appear to measure
skills that are cultivated by the practice of mindfulness, both in long-term
meditators and in relative novices. Second, increases in levels of mindfulness
appear to be related to changes in other aspects of psychological functioning
that promote well-being.
Assessment of Mindfulness as a State
The instruments discussed in previous sections measure a trait-like general
tendency to be mindful in daily life. In contrast,
Bishop et al. (2004)
view mindfulness as a state-like quality that occurs when attention is intentionally
directed to sensations, thoughts, and emotions, with an attitude of curios-
ity, openness, and acceptance. The Toronto Mindfulness Scale (TMS; Lau
et al.,
2006)
assesses attainment of a mindful state during an immediately preceding meditation session. Participants first practice a meditation exercise for about 15 minutes and then rate the extent to which they were aware
and accepting of their experiences during the exercise. This instrument
has two factors. The
curiosity
factor reflects interest and curiosity about
inner experiences and includes items such as “I was curious to see what my
mind was up to from moment to moment.” The
decentering
factor empha-
sizes awareness of experiences without identifying with them or being car-
ried away by them, and includes items such as “I experienced myself as
separate from my changing thoughts and feelings.” Findings showed good
internal consistency for each factor and significant correlations with other
measures of self-awareness. Scores increased with participation in MBSR,
and decentering scores predicted reductions in psychological symptoms and
160
Ruth A. Baer, Erin Walsh, and Emily L. B. Lykins
stress levels. This measure has good psychometric properties and is likely to
be useful in the study of mindfulness meditation. However, as the authors
note, scores reflect the experience of mindfulness during a specific medita-
tion session and may not be related to the tendency to be mindful in ordi-
nary daily life. The authors also recommend multiple assessments, because
the extent to which a mindful state was attained during a single medita-
tion session may not reflect participants’ general tendency to be mindful
while meditating, due to factors such as fatigue or stress on a particular
occasion.
Mindfulness as a state has also been assessed using experience sampling
in participants asked to carry pagers for a few weeks
(Brown & Ryan, 2003).
When paged at quasi-random intervals during each day, participants immedi-
ately responded to a subset of MAAS items asking about the extent to which
they were attending to their activity of the moment or were behaving auto-
matically. Results showed that momentary-state mindfulness was significantly
correlated with baseline levels of trait mindfulness as assessed by the original
form of the MAAS. State mindfulness also predicted higher levels of positive
emotion and autonomy and lower levels of negative emotion while engaged
in the activity of the moment.
Assessment of Closely Related Constructs
Acceptance
Acceptance has been most comprehensively described in writings on ACT
(Hayes et al., 1999; Hayes & Strosahl, 2004)
and usually refers to willingness to experience a wide range of internal experiences (such as bodily
sensations, cognitions, and emotional states) without attempting to avoid,
escape, or terminate them, even if they are unpleasant or unwanted. Accep-
tance is generally an issue when attempts to avoid or escape these experi-
ences are harmful or counterproductive. This is often true in situations that
involve competing contingencies or approach-avoidance conflicts
(Dougher,
1994).
For example, initiating conversation with a stranger may offer both reinforcing and punishing consequences (social interaction and development of a relationship versus shame or humiliation if rejected) and may
therefore elicit anxiety. Avoiding the anxiety by refraining from conversa-
tion will be counterproductive if it perpetuates loneliness. Attempts to elim-
inate the anxiety with alcohol or drugs may be harmful if these substances
contribute to socially inappropriate or ineffective behavior or maladaptive
health consequences. Thus, acceptance of feelings of anxiety (allowing them
to be present while continuing with goal-consistent behavior) may be more
adaptive.
The Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (AAQ;
Hayes, Strosahl, et al.,
2004)
is a nine-item self-report instrument whose items describe elements of experiential avoidance, including negative evaluation of and attempts to
control or avoid unpleasant internal stimuli, and inability to take constructive
action while experiencing these stimuli. If reverse scored, it serves as a mea-
sure of acceptance. Its internal consistency is adequate (alpha = 0. 70), and it
is correlated with many forms of psychopathology. A revised version by Bond
and Bunce
(2003)
includes 16 items and has two subscales: Willingness and
Chapter 9 Assessment of Mindfulness
161
Action. The first measures willingness to experience negative thoughts and
feelings and includes items such as “I try hard to avoid feeling depressed or
anxious.” The Action subscale measures ability to behave consistently with
goals and values even while having unpleasant thoughts and feelings and
includes items such as “When I feel depressed or anxious, I am unable to
take care of my responsibilities.” A revised version of the AAQ is currently in
development.
Measures based on the AAQ but modified for specific populations have
also been developed. For example, the Chronic Pain Acceptance Question-
naire (CPAQ;
McCracken, 1998; McCracken & Eccleston, 2003;
McCracken, Vowles & Eccleston,
2004)
measures recognition that pain may not change, ability to refrain from fruitless efforts to avoid or control pain, and engaging
in valued life activities despite the presence of pain. Items include, “I am
getting on with the business of living no matter what my pain level is.” Inter-
nal consistency is good (alpha = 0. 85). Scores are correlated positively with
daily activity level and improved work status and negatively with depres-
sion, anxiety, and disability, even when pain intensity is controlled. Also
derived from the AAQ, the Acceptance and Action Diabetes Questionnaire
(AADQ;
Gregg, Callaghan, Hayes, & Glenn-Lawson, 2007)
assesses acceptance of diabetes-related thoughts and feelings and ability to engage in val-
ued actions while having these experiences (e.g., “I do not take care of
my diabetes because it reminds me that I have diabetes”). Internal consis-
tency is high (alpha = 0. 94), and scores improved significantly in a group
of diabetics who participated in an ACT workshop, but not for those in
a control condition. Other measures currently in development include the
AAQ-Weight
(Lillis & Hayes, 2008)
for weight loss and weight maintenance contexts, and the Avoidance and Fusion Questionnaire for Youth (AFQ-Y;
Greco, Ball, Dew, Lambert, & Baer, 2008),
a measure for children and adolescents.
Decentering
Decentering is defined as the ability to observe one’s thoughts and feelings
as temporary events in the mind, rather than reflections of the self that are
necessarily true (Fresco, Moore, et al., 2007). It includes taking a present-
focused, nonjudgmental stance toward thoughts and feelings and accepting
them as they are
(Fresco, Segal, Buis, & Kennedy, 2007).
Decentering (also called distancing) has long been recognized as an important process in cognitive therapy for depression
(Beck, Rush, Shaw, & Emery
,
1979),
but is often viewed as a step in the process of changing thought content rather than as
an end in itself. Patients in cognitive therapy learn to adopt a decentered per-
spective on thoughts by viewing them as ideas to be tested, rather than truths
(Hollon & Beck
,
1979).
However, they then go on to dispute distorted thoughts and generate more rational ones. Several authors have suggested
that decentering alone may be the central ingredient in the effectiveness
of cognitive therapy in preventing relapse of depression (Ingram & Hol-
lon,
1986;
Segal et al., 2002).
It is a central ingredient in MBCT, which uses the intensive practice of mindfulness meditation to teach decentering, which in turn reduces rumination and lowers the likelihood of
relapse.
162
Ruth A. Baer, Erin Walsh, and Emily L. B. Lykins
Decentering can be measured with two recently developed tools. The Mea-
sure of Awareness and Coping in Autobiographical Memory (MACAM; Moore,
Hayhurst, & Teasdale,
1996)
is a vignette-based, semistructured clinical interview in which participants are asked to imagine themselves in several mildly
depressing situations and to feel the feelings that would be elicited. They
are then asked to recall specific occasions from their own lives that the
vignettes bring to mind and to describe these occasions in detail, including
their feelings and how they responded to them. Responses are tape recorded,
and trained raters then code the responses for the presence of decentering
or awareness of thoughts and feelings as separate from the self. Teasdale
et al.
(2002)
found that decentering scores were higher for a group of never-depressed adults than for a previously depressed group. Previously depressed
patients who completed MBCT showed larger increases in decentering than a
control group who received treatment as usual. Finally, lower baseline levels
of decentering predicted earlier relapse following treatment for depression
with either cognitive therapy or medication. Overall, these findings support
the idea that the ability to adopt a decentered perspective on thoughts and
feelings is centrally related to recovery from depression and prevention of
relapse.
Although the MACAM appears to have good psychometric properties, it
is time consuming and difficult to use. For this reason, Fresco, Moore, et al.
(2007) conducted a psychometric evaluation of the experiences question-
naire (EQ), a rationally derived self-report instrument designed by Teasdale
to assess decentering and rumination. Analyses by Fresco et al. (in press)
yielded an 11-item decentering factor, which includes items such as “I can
observe unpleasant feelings without being drawn into them” and “I can sep-
arate myself from my thoughts and feelings.” The EQ showed good inter-
nal consistency and was correlated in expected directions with measures
of depressive rumination, experiential avoidance, emotion regulation, and
depression. Depressed patients showed lower levels of decentering than
healthy controls (Fresco et al., in press). In a second study, Fresco, Segal,